国产日韩欧美一区二区三区三州_亚洲少妇熟女av_久久久久亚洲av国产精品_波多野结衣网站一区二区_亚洲欧美色片在线91_国产亚洲精品精品国产优播av_日本一区二区三区波多野结衣 _久久国产av不卡

?

Human memory across nations and the potential for optimizing learning

2012-04-29 12:48:02石妮
都市家教·上半月 2012年6期
關鍵詞:教導處桂林市學歷

You are studying a foreign language and you must learn 30 words from yesterdays class. How can you achieve this with the fewest number of tries and retain the words for the long run?

Perhaps you may learn the words in three sessions of 10 minutes each if you group them under pertinent categories, visualize their characteristics, invent mnemonics, link them to related concepts. But to memorize them in a hodgepodge order you may need 5 20-minute sessions – and later forget more. And overall, you may recall more words a year later if you rehearse after one month than if you rehearse them after one week.

Will the same learning methods work for the languages of the Pygmies as well as for French? the inhabitants of Nauru or the Canadian First Nations? High-income countries as well as developing countries?

They probably will. All humans belong to one species, so despite individual and cultural differences, we process information in the same general ways. Our memory is like a huge bottle with a very narrow neck. We can retain infinite amounts of information, but at any given moment we can process very little of it. This feature influences our thinking and the way our languages are made. So, languages and scripts appear to be very different across the earth, but in fact they conform to memory specifications.

The commonalities of human cognition can help make the education of the poor in low-income countries more efficient. Instructional methods that dovetail the ways people are set up to retain information may educate more students and save money and time.

The learning efficiency concept has received little attention thus far, partly because high-income countries have little need for it in the early grades. (Studies like Pashler et al. 2007 aim at higher knowledge levels.) In higher-income countries, curricula and timetables have some redundancy to allow for repeated learning opportunities, so even lower-performing children acquire basic skills. But in poor countries instructional time is often wasted, and students have little opportunity to catch up on something they miss. Trained teachers are scarce, and schools may be already used in three shifts. It becomes important therefore, to use methods that help students encode and retain as much as possible from every instructional hour.

Such methods must be identified, promoted, and put to use. Some hypotheses exist, but methods must be specified for efficiency. For example, how can poorer students be taught to recognize all letters in, say 15 days rather than 30? Systematic instruction of letter analogies probably speeds up retention, but by how much, compared to alternatives? How often should activities change to avoid attention span loss? It is known that certain movements consolidate memory, but which ones will facilitate retention of specific items? These issues seem immaterial where students have abundant opportunities to learn. But in countries where student lives and millions of dollars are at stake, such details may help move some students from early dropout to basic skills acquisition. To retain new items, we must know the prerequisites. Therefore to progress meaningfully to higher grades, efficiency in acquiring basic skills is a crucial topic.

Interestingly, learning efficiency is a concept that intersects with educational economics. Economists study the internal efficiency of school systems as portrayed by student flows: dropout, repetition, attainment rates. The unit of measurement is the student, but the underlying construct is learning efficiency. Perhaps, retention of information per unit of instructional time could be quantified. This would be more useful for the early grades when information is fairly concrete and essential. The uniformity of human cognition would facilitate generalizations.

But in an age where individual differences and cultural sensitivities are celebrated, emphasis on underlying uniformities may arouse philosophical questions. Individual attention to students would be highly desirable, but the schools of the poor give limited opportunities for this. What is the role of our cognitive commonalities vis a vis our cultural differences? Readers views would be greatly appreciated.

作者簡介:

石妮(1978-),女,1978年生,廣西桂林人,廣西桂林市樂群小學教導處主任,學歷:本科,主要從事小學語文教學研究。

猜你喜歡
教導處桂林市學歷
學歷造假引發(fā)法律糾紛
公民與法治(2022年4期)2022-08-03 08:20:42
六點一百分
對“慢孩子”,評價更應柔情
廣西壯族自治區(qū)桂林市桂林中學
桂林市供水管網(wǎng)在線水力模型的建立與應用初探
供水技術(2021年2期)2021-07-16 07:30:10
對“慢孩子”,評價更應柔情
科教新報(2020年29期)2020-08-11 20:02:18
學歷軍備競賽,可以休矣
教導處在哪兒
加強教導處建設促進素質教育發(fā)展
珍稀瀕危植物在桂林市市區(qū)園林綠化中的應用
宝清县| 田东县| 邯郸市| 方正县| 运城市| 会泽县| 井冈山市| 五原县| 新竹市| 井研县| 望都县| 中方县| 肃北| 柳河县| 韶山市| 江达县| 定州市| 长宁区| 宿州市| 驻马店市| 山阳县| 阳泉市| 高尔夫| 尉犁县| 弥渡县| 深圳市| 郸城县| 盖州市| 西林县| 宝清县| 平武县| 浮梁县| 张掖市| 吴川市| 古交市| 岳阳市| 东城区| 天长市| 桐柏县| 宁津县| 常山县|